Indoor environment quality (IEQ) factors: Difference between revisions

From Opasnet
Jump to navigation Jump to search
Line 111: Line 111:
Rehousing|faints/dizziness|12.6|not applicable|yes/no||-5.7||Pettricrew et al. 2009  
Rehousing|faints/dizziness|12.6|not applicable|yes/no||-5.7||Pettricrew et al. 2009  
Rehousing|difficulty in sleeping|8.6|not applicable|yes/no||-17.4||Pettricrew et al. 2009
Rehousing|difficulty in sleeping|8.6|not applicable|yes/no||-17.4||Pettricrew et al. 2009
Rehousing|sinus trouble/catarh||Other|yes/no||-4.7||Pettricrew et al. 2009
Rehousing|sinus trouble/catarh|-0.3|Other|yes/no||-4.7||Pettricrew et al. 2009


Renting a house/lacking own accommodation |poor health rate||not applicable|yes or no|OR|1.48(1.31-1.68)||Pollack et al 2004 </t2b>
Renting a house/lacking own accommodation |poor health rate||not applicable|yes or no|OR|1.48(1.31-1.68)||Pollack et al 2004 </t2b>
Line 170: Line 170:
{{defend|# |Correct parts include:  
{{defend|# |Correct parts include:  
*"exposure unit"
*"exposure unit"
*many of the responses and their values, however these values should be moved to column "ERF". For example, "painful joint" is a correct response, its "ERF parameter" is -8.7 as you have stated (but move this to the right place!). However, the respective exposure is not "housing dampness", instead it is here "rehousing", since the values are given in relation to it (table 5).|--[[User:Marjo|Marjo]] 17:34, 11 February 2013 (EET)}}
*many of the responses and their values, however these values should be moved to column "ERF". For example, "painful joint" is a correct response, its "ERF parameter" is -8.7 as you have stated (but move this to the right place!). However, the respective exposure is not "housing dampness", instead it is here "rehousing", since the values are given in relation to it (table 5).|--[[User:Marjo|Marjo]] 17:34, 11 February 2013 (EET)}}{{comment|# |table updated|--[[User:Matthew|Matthew]] 13:12, 13 February 2013 (EET)}}





Revision as of 11:12, 13 February 2013



Question

What established or possible indoor environment quality (IEQ) factors exist? What kind of dose-responses have been defined for them?

Answer

You have error(s) in your data:

You have invalid number of data cells in row 102

Indoor environment quality (IEQ) factors: Difference between revisions(-)
ObsExposure metricResponseResponse metricExposure routeExposure unitERF parameterERFSignificanceDescription/Reference
1Visible dampness and/or mold or mold odorRespiratory health effectInhalationyes/noORseveral, see Note 1Note 1
2Dampness and/or moldGeneral health problemperceptionyes/noincreased risk of health problems %64%Evans et al (2000)
3Dampness and/or moldMental health problemsperceptionOR1.39(1.44-2.78)Shenassa et al. 2007
4Dampness and/or moldSelf-assessed health poorerInhalation, Other?Note 2
5Dampness and/or moldMental health problemsInhalation, dermal and ingestionyes/noOR1.76 (1.17-2.66)Hopton and Hunt (1996)
6Chronic illness Mental health problemsnot applicableyes/noOR1.99 (1.32-3.02)Hopton and Hunt (1996)
7Living with children under 16 y Mental health problemsnot applicableyes/noOR1.75 (1.15-2.68)Hopton and Hunt (1996)
8Living in a low income household Mental health problemsnot applicableyes/noOR1.61 (1.06-2.44)Hopton and Hunt (1996)
9Respondent unemployed Mental health problemsnot applicableyes/noOR1.55 (0.99-2.42)Hopton and Hunt (1996)
10Living in flat instead of houseUpper respitory infectionInhalationyes/noD.Fanning (1967)
11Living in flat instead of houseMinor mental health problemsnot applicableyes/noD.Fanning (1967)
12Living in flat instead of houseMorbiditynot applicableyes/noIncreased morbidity (%)57%D.Fanning (1967)
13Living in ground floorPsychoneurotic disordernot applicableyes/noIncreased risk of psychoneurotic disorder (%)6,3%D.Fanning (1967)
14Living in 1st floorPsychoneurotic disordernot applicableyes/noIncreased risk of psychoneurotic disorder (%)6,7%D.Fanning (1967)
15Living in 2nd floorPsychoneurotic disordernot applicableyes/noIncreased risk of psychoneurotic disorder (%)10,9%D.Fanning (1967)
16Living in 3rd floorPsychoneurotic disordernot applicableyes/noIncreased risk of psychoneurotic disorder (%)12,7%D.Fanning (1967)
17Wood smokeRespiratory health effectInhalationNote 3, Note 4
18Wood smokeIrritation of eyes and mucosa
19Wood smokeRespiratory health effectInhalation
20Wood smokeOdour problemsInhalation
21Wood smokeComfort of housing
22Wood smokeChronic infectionsInhalation
23Wood smokeCancerInhalation
24Tobacco smokeRespiratory health effectInhalation
25Tobacco smokeIrritation of eyes and mucosa
26Tobacco smokeRespiratory health effect
27Tobacco smokeOdour problemsInhalation
28Tobacco smokeComfort of housing
29Tobacco smokeChronic infectionsInhalation
30Tobacco smokeCancer
31VOCsirritation symptoms etc.
32CO2headache, tiredness etc.
33COheadache, tiredness etc.
34Insufficient air exchangeHeadache
35Insufficient air exchangeTiredness
36Insufficient air exchangeDecreased ability to concentrate
37Insufficient air exchangeFeeling of fug
38Thermal conditions; heatTiredness
39Thermal conditions; heatDecreased ability to concentrate
40Thermal conditions; heatIncreased respiratory symptoms
41Thermal conditions; heatFeeling of dryness
42Thermal conditions; heatComfort of housing
43Thermal comfort (draught or cold)Mental health problemsNote 2
44Thermal comfort (heat or cold)DepressionNote 2
45Thermal comfort (heat or cold; general perception of thermal problems)Self-assessed health poorerNote 2
46Thermal conditions (cold)Feeling of draught
47Thermal conditions (cold)Comfort of housing
48NoiseHearing injury
49NoiseSleep disturbance
50NoiseStress
51NoiseComfort of housing
52Proximity to trafficMortality(?)
53RadonLung cancerNote 5
54Relative humidity
55PMmortalityNote 3
56PMchronic bronchitis
57PMlung cancer
58Reduced space (house/flat)DepressionNote 2
59Reduced space (house/flat)Mental health problemsNote 2
60Reduced space (house/flat)Self-assessed health poorerNote 2
61GardenDepressionNote 2
62Floor levelMental health problemsNote 2
63OvercrowdingMental health problemsNote 2
64OvercrowdingSelf assessed health poorerNote 2
65Sensory IAQVarious health and well-being parameters
66Maternal employmentMaltreatment of ChildrenOtherno/yesOR2.82 (1.59-5.00)Sidebotham et al. 2002
67No. of house moves in previous 5 yearsMaltreatment of ChildrenOther2-3 vs. 0-1OR1.32 (0.77-2.27)Sidebotham et al. 2002
68No. of house moves in previous 5 yearsMaltreatment of ChildrenOther4 or more vs. 0-1OR2.81 (1.59-4.96)Sidebotham et al. 2002
69Overcrowed accomodationMaltreatment of ChildrenOtheryes/noOR2.16 (1.27-3.70)Sidebotham et al. 2002
70AccomodationMaltreatment of ChildrenOtherCouncil vs. owned/mortgargedOR7.65 (3.30-17.75)Sidebotham et al. 2002
71AccomodationMaltreatment of ChildrenOtherRented vs. owned/mortgargedOR4.47 (1.82-10.98)Sidebotham et al. 2002
72Social Network Score < 21Maltreatment of ChildrenOtheryes/noOR3.09 (1.84-5.19)Sidebotham et al. 2002
73Paternal employementMaltreatment of ChildrenOtherno/yesOR2.33 (1.43-3.77)Sidebotham et al. 2002
74Car useMaltreatment of ChildrenOtherno/yesOR2.33 (1.41-3.83)Sidebotham et al. 2002
75No. of deprivation indicatorsMaltreatment of ChildrenOther1 vs. 0OR9.58 (2.64-34.81)Note6; Sidebotham et al. 2002
76No. of deprivation indicatorsMaltreatment of ChildrenOther2 vs. 0OR23.44 (6.61-83.15)Note6; Sidebotham et al. 2002
77No. of deprivation indicatorsMaltreatment of ChildrenOther3 vs. 0OR59.30 (17.52-200.76)Note6; Sidebotham et al. 2002
78No. of deprivation indicatorsMaltreatment of ChildrenOther4 vs. 0OR111.36 (32.31-383.801)Note6; Sidebotham et al. 2002
79House dampnessSmoking38.2Inhalation otheryes/noPacker et al. 1994
80House dampnessuse of low fat milk 40.0Digestion, otheryes/noPacker et al. 1994
81House dampnessExercise 3 last week 15.4Otheryes/noPacker et al. 1994
82House dampnessBody mass index >2534.2Otheryes/noPacker et al. 1994
83House dampnessAlcohol over limit14.3Drinkingyes/noPacker et al. 1994
84House dampnessEnergy38.5Otheryes/noPacker et al. 1994
85House dampnessSocial isolation22.7Otheryes/noPacker et al. 1994
86House dampnessSleep40.5Otheryes/noPacker et al. 1994
87House dampnessEmotional reactions39.5Otheryes/noPacker et al. 1994
88House dampnessPhysical mobility16.7otheryes/noPacker et al. 1994
89House dampnessPain14.4otheryes/noPacker et al. 1994
90Smokingchronic respiratory diseaseInhalationyes/noOR4.36(2.46-7.74)Blackman et al. (2001)
91Dampnesschronic respiratory diseaseInhalationyes/noOR2.10(1.36-3.50)Blackman et al. (2001)
92Unwaged householdchronic respiratory diseaseotheryes/noOR1.73(1.24-2.41)Blackman et al. (2001)
93Unsafe neighborhoodmental health problemsotheryes/noOR2.35(1.41-3.92)Blackman et al. (2001)
94Chronic respiratory problemsmental health problemsotheryes/noOR2.35(1.50-3.69)Blackman et al. (2001)
95Draughtsmental health problemsotheryes/noOR2.28(1.41-3.69)Blackman et al. (2001)
96Rehousingpalpitation/breathlessness0.8otheryes/no-7.8Pettricrew et al. 2009
97Rehousingpersistence cough-11.0Inhalationyes/no-2.1pettricrew et al. 2009
98Rehousingpainful joint32.9Inhalationyes/no-8.7Pettricrew et al. 2009
99Rehousingfaints/dizziness12.6not applicableyes/no-5.7Pettricrew et al. 2009
100Rehousingdifficulty in sleeping8.6not applicableyes/no-17.4Pettricrew et al. 2009
101Rehousingsinus trouble/catarh-0.3Otheryes/no-4.7Pettricrew et al. 2009
102


Note 1 ERF of indoor dampness on respiratory health effects

Note 2 WP6 well-being report (password-protected)

Note 3 ERF of PM2.5 on mortality in general population

Note 4 Concentration-response to PM2.5

Note 5 Health impact of radon in Europe

Note 6 Indicators of deprication: overcrowded accommodation, accomodation ownership, paternal employment, car use


⇤--#: . Comments on Hopton and Hunt (1996):

  • Row 5: Are you sure that the only possible exposure route is inhalation?
  • Rows 6 to 9: Instead of "no", exposure route should be "not applicable".
  • Rows 5 to 9: Use periods instead of commas as decimal points. --Marjo 10:22, 4 February 2013 (EET) (type: truth; paradigms: science: attack)

←--#: . Comments have been considered. --Juho Kutvonen 13:52, 4 February 2013 (EET) (type: truth; paradigms: science: defence)

⇤--#: . Comments on Sidebotham et al. (2002)

  • Rows 59, 66 and 67 are filled correctly. What comes to rows 60 to 65, small but essential changes should be done in columns "exposure metric" and "exposure unit". An example: "exposure metric" of row 60 should be "2 to 3 house moves in previous 5 years" and the respective "exposure unit" should be "medium vs. low". Based on this example, can you figure out the correct structures of rows 61 to 65? --Marjo 10:41, 4 February 2013 (EET) (type: truth; paradigms: science: attack)

←--#: . Good revisions, you have the right idea. However, some minor modifications would be appropriate: in row 62 "exposure unit" can simply be "yes/no", as the accomodation either is overcrowded or is not; no other possibilities exist. In row 65 the "exposure metric" should be "Social network score < 21" and "exposure unit" again simply "yes/no". --Marjo 15:44, 6 February 2013 (EET) (type: truth; paradigms: science: defence)


⇤--#: . Comments on Packer et al. (1994)

  • In the paper of Packer et al. (1994) no ORs are given. Instead, they have measured prevalences. Therefore, "response metric" should be "prevalence" and "ERF parameter" should be "percentage unit change".
  • Row 69: According the Table 4, "exposure metric" is damp housing and "response" is "smoking". Based on this, can you figure out the correct structures of rows 70 to 73? --Marjo 11:05, 4 February 2013 (EET) (type: truth; paradigms: science: attack)

----#: . we updated the data --Soroushm 23:25, 10 February 2013 (EET) (type: truth; paradigms: science: comment) ⇤--#: . I see that you have updated the data using the table 1. Unfortunately, that is not correct. You should update the data using tables 4 and 8, where relationships of dampness and various health-related endpoints are shown. --Marjo 15:27, 11 February 2013 (EET) (type: truth; paradigms: science: attack)----#: . Table updated --Adnank 10:19, 13 February 2013 (EET) (type: truth; paradigms: science: comment)

⇤--#: . Comments on Blackman et al. (2001)

  • "Response metric" should describe how the response was measured: number of cases, incidence, prevalence,... I see that you have filled "response metric" boxes according earlier versions of this table, unfortunately, "response metric" was not used correctly there. The information you now have in boxes "response" and "response metric" all belongs to "response". You could do the following: decide and formulate the most accurate responses and put them into "response" -boxes and empty the "response metric" -boxes. If you can define the response metric, i.e. number of cases, incidence, prevalence etc.. used in the article, you can put it into "response metric" box. --Marjo 14:39, 4 February 2013 (EET) (type: truth; paradigms: science: attack)

⇤--#: . Don´t you think that the most likely exposure route in case of smoking and chronic respiratory disease as well as in case of dampness and chronic respiratory disease would be inhalation? --Marjo 16:31, 7 February 2013 (EET) (type: truth; paradigms: science: attack)

----#: . Data tables updated, and correct, most likely exposure route to those exposure metrics would be inhalation --Jukka Hirvonen 09:11, 11 February 2013 (EET) (type: truth; paradigms: science: comment)

⇤--#: . Comments on Fanning (1967)

  • This article does not express ORs, which makes it a bit tricky in terms of this exercise. Anyhow, the idea is to find numerical value for ERF to be added into table. At least for morbidity a numerical value can be found in the article, although it is not OR. Can you find it?
  • If no numerical value can be found for the two other responses, they should be removed. Instead, you could try to put the data of Table VIII of the article into the IEQ table.
  • Exposure route can not be "neurosis" or "common sickness". I suggest exposure route in these cases is "not applicable". --Marjo 17:29, 8 February 2013 (EET) (type: truth; paradigms: science: attack)

←--#: . I see that you have made good corrections in the IEQ table. Still something:

  • "ERF parameter" should be "percentage unit change" in all cases.
  • Use periods instead of commas as decimal separator. --Marjo 16:13, 12 February 2013 (EET) (type: truth; paradigms: science: defence)


⇤--#: . Comments on Petticrew et al. 2009

  • You have both incorrect and correct parts here. Your "exposure metrics" are mostly wrong. I admit, this is tricky, as what you have written as "exposure metrics" could well be that. However, here "exposure metric" in all cases is "rehousing" for which a number of "responses" are described in tables 3,4,5 and 6. As there are so many responses, I suggest that you select only 6 of them to be included into the IEQ table above. Select those 6 responses you consider most interesting.
  • Your article does not express ORs, instead prevalences and changes in them are given. Therefore, the "response metric" is here "prevalence" and "ERF parameter" is "percentage unit change". --Marjo 17:34, 11 February 2013 (EET) (type: truth; paradigms: science: attack)

←--#: . Correct parts include:

  • "exposure unit"
  • many of the responses and their values, however these values should be moved to column "ERF". For example, "painful joint" is a correct response, its "ERF parameter" is -8.7 as you have stated (but move this to the right place!). However, the respective exposure is not "housing dampness", instead it is here "rehousing", since the values are given in relation to it (table 5). --Marjo 17:34, 11 February 2013 (EET) (type: truth; paradigms: science: defence)----#: . table updated --Matthew 13:12, 13 February 2013 (EET) (type: truth; paradigms: science: comment)


Thomasa and Joshuan Evans et al. (2000). [1]

Rationale

An example for RefTag functionality: Pope et al. (2002) [2]

john agyemang and emmanuel Shenassa et al. (2007). [3]

Juho Kutvonen and Salla Mönkkönen Hopton and Hunt (1996) [4]

Isabell Rumrich and Stefania Caporaso Sidebotham et al. (2002) [5]

Soroush Majlesi and Adnan Ahmad Packer et al. (1994) [6]

Jukka Hirvonen and Sami Rissanen Blackman et al. (2001) [7]

Niklas Holopainen and Kasperi Juntunen Fanning D. M. et al. (1967) [8]

Matthew Adeboye and Adedayo Mofikoya Petticrew et al. (2009) [9]

Precision and Plausability of Hopton and Hunt (1996)

- Reporting bias: Perhaps ít´s difficult to use subjective data due to reporting bias. This is because people may answer in different ways or they don´t answer at all. In addition, people experience household conditions differently.←--#: . Good points. --Marjo 14:50, 4 February 2013 (EET) (type: truth; paradigms: science: defence)

- Possible confounding variables were controlled. ----#: . Can you give examples of the confounding variables mentioned in the paper? --Marjo 14:50, 4 February 2013 (EET) (type: truth; paradigms: science: comment)←--#: . Sociodemographic and economic variables, e.g. age and income. --Juho Kutvonen 12:23, 6 February 2013 (EET) (type: truth; paradigms: science: defence)

- Selection bias: The sample is clearly not representative of the general population and therefore the analysis focuses on differences within the sample. Thus it´s worth considering if the results can be generalized to whole population.←--#: . Good points. --Marjo 14:50, 4 February 2013 (EET) (type: truth; paradigms: science: defence)


Precision and Plausability of Sidebotham et al. (2002)

- Maltreatment is defined and measured as registration for physical injury, neglect, sexual abuse, emotional abuse. That way all maltreatments, which are not registred are not taken into account.

- The measurement of the social class is not too accurate, because no allowance for nonworking mothers and no parental social class allocated for single mothers can be applied.

- The nature of relationship with child maltreatment is complex (confounder, cultural values, etc). That causes problems finding an association or causality between an exposure factor and maltreatment. Moreover, maltreatment has different definition in different cultural groups.

- The parental income is not measured directly, but car ownership as a proxy indicator and the receipt of welfare payment are used.

- Controlling for social factors was done.

- Large amount of prospectively data are collected and used in in the study, which is a clear strength.

- The participation is lower among the maltreated group, which might influence the outcome of the statistical analysis or bias the results of the study.

- The risk of social bias and no way of measuring the effect of such bias. A social bias can be defined as a prejudgement of a specific social group. In this case, it might be that those, who collected the data might have expectations, that parents which lower or higher social background are more prone to maltreat their child and let this expectation influence their interpretation of the results. This is not very likely here, though, because all parameters which were used for the analysis can me measured and there is not much freedome for interpretation.


----#: . You have listed correct points that may affect precision and plausibility of the ERF; well done. However, it would be easier for the reader if you would use full sentences or otherwise would explain a bit more in detail how these issues affect the precision and plausibity of ERF.

  • What is meant with "social bias" here? --Marjo 15:06, 4 February 2013 (EET) (type: truth; paradigms: science: comment)

----#: . We added explanations. --Isabell Rumrich 09:58, 7 February 2013 (EET) (type: truth; paradigms: science: comment)


Precision and Plausability of Packer et al. (1994)

- health problems: possibility of headache, mental problems, emotional reactions, social isolation and pain.

- social factors: unemployment, single parent, lone adult and unemployment with sickness or disability

- lifestyle: consumption of alcohol and smoking

----#: . It might be helpful for the reader if you would use full sentences in order to explain how the above issues affect the precision and plausibility of ERF. --Marjo 15:28, 4 February 2013 (EET) (type: truth; paradigms: science: comment)

- it is still difficult to understand the housing condition because none of the studies are complete and detailed so that direct comparison with the questions cannot be made and measurements of parameters, potential confounding factors as well as clear dose-response relationship should be adjusted for example physical effect of damp is responsible for muscle tension, backache and headache but on the other hand the study poins out that there is a strong relationship between damp housing and adverse health impact. ----#: . I see that the two last points are in concordance with each other. --Marjo 15:28, 4 February 2013 (EET) (type: truth; paradigms: science: comment)

Precision and Plausability of Blackman et al. (2001)

- Bias in respondents answers to realistically evaluate their and family members health.←--#: . Good. --Marjo 16:25, 7 February 2013 (EET) (type: truth; paradigms: science: defence)

- Some housings that where targets on first survey were demolished during second survey.

- No data from comparison neighbourhood without renewal to back up observed health changes after renewal program. ←--#: . Good point. --Marjo 16:25, 7 February 2013 (EET) (type: truth; paradigms: science: defence)

- Relationship between dampness, draughts and mental health is uncertain, because the mechanism is unknown.←--#: . Again good, although you could specify this. Is it so that associations have been found but the mechanisms are unclear? --Marjo 16:25, 7 February 2013 (EET) (type: truth; paradigms: science: defence)

- Multivariate analysis using regression model was used to control variables, such as economic, housing, respiratory and mental health related to increase plausability----#: . So in contrast to the previous points, this increases the plausibility of ERF, is this what you mean? --Marjo 16:25, 7 February 2013 (EET) (type: truth; paradigms: science: comment)

----#: . Precisions and plausabilities updated --Jukka Hirvonen 09:30, 11 February 2013 (EET) (type: truth; paradigms: science: comment)

Precision and Plausability of D. Fanning (1967)

- The study is so old that the exposures and responses are real but the accuracy is quite poor. The basics are almost same as today but measurement techniques are so old that the results are not comparable to modern results. ⇤--#: . Which specific measurement techniques do you mean? For example, they have measured first attendances by general practitioners, and I don´t think the accuracy of counting has changed significantly. --Marjo 18:08, 8 February 2013 (EET) (type: truth; paradigms: science: attack)←--#: . However, I agree with you that the oldness of study is a bit striking. Probably today many other parameters in addition to those used in the article would be measured when conducting this kind of study. --Marjo 18:08, 8 February 2013 (EET) (type: truth; paradigms: science: defence)

- The study has considered the difference between children and adults.←--#: . Good point. --Marjo 18:08, 8 February 2013 (EET) (type: truth; paradigms: science: defence)

- The study has not considered the differences between different flats and houses. They have only categories for houses and flats but the differences between houses are not considered. This causes bias to the study.----#: . Well, it is possible. However, it is always a question of e.g. resources how specific and detailed a study can be. Maybe they could more apparently mention whether there were any significant dissimilarities between houses. --Marjo 18:08, 8 February 2013 (EET) (type: truth; paradigms: science: comment)


Precision and Plausability of Petticrew et al. (2009)

- Data collection at the three occassions in the intervention group before moving, one year after moving and 2 years after moving to the social housing gives strenght to the study in analysing changes in the housing circumstances and in neighbourhood.

- Recruitment into the study was discussed by the landlord to the tenant once they have accepted the housing offer which dosn't gives the RSL direct contact with the participant though this serves as a way of good recruitments but it dose not guarantee the authenticity of the data collected. e.g RSL couldn't supply the number of people who refuse to participate in the study to the SHARP research team.

- Broad range of adult household categories in the intervention group which was used as a base for recruiting the comparism group stenghthen the study. (family households, with children under age of sixteen years, older households where the respondents and adult members of the households were of pensionable age, and adult households with a combination of relationships, including parents with children atleast 16 years of age, people unrelated to one and another and couples )

- Qualitative and quantitative findings were only presented for 1 year(wave 2) in the study which dose not proof if the effects are sustained and probabely if differences in health outcomes occur at two years in the intervention and comparism groups.

- recollection bias may occur during interview if participant in the groups if they can not recall adequately past occurences relating to health, housing and neighbourhood questions after one year and two years of movement to the new house.

- Bias in subsequent analysis can also occur if there is any significant changes in the groups associated with self reported health.

←--#: . Good points and thorough work! By checking the spelling you could increase the elegancy of your work. --Marjo 17:46, 11 February 2013 (EET) (type: truth; paradigms: science: defence)----#: . updated --Matthew 12:43, 13 February 2013 (EET) (type: truth; paradigms: science: comment)


Dependencies

Formula

See also

Keywords

References

  1. Evans J, Hyndman S, Stewart-Brown S, Smith D, & Petersen S, (2000). An epidemiological study of the relative importance of damp housing in relation to adult health. J Epidemiol Community Health 2000;54:677–686..
  2. *Pope CA III, Burnett RT, Thun MJ, Calle EE, Krewski D, Ito K & Thurston KD (2002). Lung cancer, cardiopulmonary mortality, and long-term exposure to fine particulate air pollution. JAMA 287(9), 1132-1141.
  3. * Shenassa et al. (2007)Dampness and Mold in the Home and Depression: An Examination of Mold-Related Illness and Perceived Control of One’s Home as Possible Depression Pathways. America Journal of Public Health 2007 97(10): 1893–1899
  4. *Hopton J.L. and Hunt S.M.(1996). Housing conditions and mental health in a disadvantaged area in Scotland. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health 1996;50:56-61
  5. *Sidebotham et al. (2002). Child maltreatment in the “Children of the Nineties:” deprivation, class, and social networks in a UK sample.Child Abuse and Neglect 2002;26:1243-1259
  6. *Packer et al. Damp housing and adult health: results from a lifestyle study in Worcester, England.Journal of epidemiology and community health 1994;48(6):555–559
  7. *Blackman T, Harvey J, Lawrence M & Simon A. (2001). Neighbourhood renewal and health: evidence from a local case study. Health & Place 7(2001), 93-103.
  8. *Fanning D. M. (1967). Families in flats. British Medical Journal 4(1967), 382-386.
  9. *Petticrew et al. (2009). Quantitative and qualitative evaluation of the short-term outcomes of housing and neighbourhood renewal. BMC public health 2009;9:415

Related files

<mfanonymousfilelist></mfanonymousfilelist>

Indoor environment quality (IEQ) factors. Opasnet . [1]. Accessed 24 Nov 2024.